Anggoro, F. K., Waxman, S. R., & Medin, D. L. (2008). Naming practices and the acquisition of key biological concepts: Evidence from English and Indonesian. <em>Psychological Science</em>, <em>19</em>(4), 314–319. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02086.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02086.x</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02086.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02086.x</a></pub-id>
Astuti, R., Solomon, G. E. A., Carey, S., Ingold, T., & Miller, P. (2004). Constraints on Conceptual Development: A Case Study of the Acquisition of Folkbiological and Folksociological Knowledge in Madagascar. <em>Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development</em>, 69(3), I-161. Retrieved from <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="http://www.jstor.org/stable/3701405">http://www.jstor.org/stable/3701405</ext-link>
Backscheider, A. G., Shatz, M., & Gelman, S. A. (1993). Preschoolers’ ability to distinguish living kinds as a function of regrowth. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>64</em>(4), 1242–1257. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04198.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04198.x</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04198.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04198.x</a></pub-id>
Bailenson, J. N., Shum, M. S., Atran, S., Medin, D. L., & Coley, J. D. (2002). A bird’s eye view: Biological categorization and reasoning within and across cultures. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>84</em>(1), 1–53. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00011-2" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00011-2</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00011-2" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00011-2</a></pub-id>
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Macdonald, R., Serratrice, L., Melville, J., & Gattis, M. (2017). Plant yourself where language blooms: Direct experience of nature changes how parents and children talk about nature. <em>Children, Youth and Environments</em>, <em>27</em>(2), 110–124. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.27.2.0110" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.7721/chilyoutenvi.27.2.0110</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.27.2.0110" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.7721/chilyoutenvi.27.2.0110</a></pub-id>
Carey, S. (1999). Sources of conceptual change. In: E. K. Scholnick, K. Nelson, S. A. Gelman, & P. H. Miller (Eds.) <em>Conceptual development: Piaget’s legacy</em> (pp 293–326). New York: Psychology Press.
Crowley, K., & Jacobs, M. (2002). Building islands of expertise in everyday family activity. in G. Leinhardt, K. Crowley, & K. Knutson (Eds). <em>Learning conversations in museums</em> (pp 333-356). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
Fender, J. G., & Crowley, K. (2007). How parent explanation changes what children learn from everyday scientific thinking. <em>Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology</em>, <em>28</em>(3), 189–210. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2007.02.007" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.appdev.2007.02.007</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2007.02.007" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.appdev.2007.02.007</a></pub-id>
Fisher, A. V., Godwin, K. E., & Matlen, B. J. (2015). Development of inductive generalization with familiar categories. <em>Psychonomic Bulletin & Review</em>, <em>22</em>(5), 1149–1173. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0816-5" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3758/s13423-015-0816-5</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0816-5" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3758/s13423-015-0816-5</a></pub-id>
Geerdts, M. S., Van de Walle, G. A., & LoBue, V. (2015). Parent-child conversations about animals in informal learning environments. <em>Visitor Studies</em>, <em>18</em>(1), 39–63. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/10645578.2015.1016366" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/10645578.2015.1016366</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/10645578.2015.1016366" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/10645578.2015.1016366</a></pub-id>
Gelman, S. A. (2003). <em>The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought</em>. <em>Oxford Series in Cognitive Develoment.</em> New York: Oxford University Press<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Gelman, S. A., & Kremer, K. E. (1991). Understanding natural cause: Children’s explanations of how objects and their properties originate. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>62</em>(2), 396–414. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01540.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01540.x</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01540.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01540.x</a></pub-id>
Gelman, S. A., & Markman, E. M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>23</em>(3), 183–209. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90034-X" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(86)90034-X</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90034-X" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(86)90034-X</a></pub-id>
Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (1991). Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>38</em>(3), 213–244. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90007-Q" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(91)90007-Q</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90007-Q" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(91)90007-Q</a></pub-id>
Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., Rosengren, K. S., Hartman, E., & Pappas, A. (1998). Beyond labeling: The role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. <em>Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development</em>, <em>63</em>(1), i-148. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/1166211" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/1166211</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/1166211" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/1166211</a></pub-id>
Gelman, S. A., Raman, L., & Gentner, D. (2009). Effects of language and similarity on comparison processing. <em>Language Learning and Development</em>, <em>5</em>(3), 147–171. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/15475440902824079" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/15475440902824079</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/15475440902824079" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/15475440902824079</a></pub-id>
Gelman, S. A., Ware, E. A., Kleinberg, F., Manczak, E. M., & Stilwell, S. M. (2014). Individual differences in children’s and parents’ generic language. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>85</em>(3), 924–940. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12187" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/cdev.12187</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12187" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/cdev.12187</a></pub-id>
Gobbo, C., & Chi, M. (1986). How knowledge is structured and used by expert and novice children. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>1</em>(3), 221–237. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(86)80002-8" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0885-2014(86)80002-8</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(86)80002-8" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0885-2014(86)80002-8</a></pub-id>
Graham, S. A., Namy, L. L., Gentner, D., & Meagher, K. (2010). The role of comparison in preschoolers’ novel object categorization. <em>Journal of Experimental Child Psychology</em>, <em>107</em>(3), 280–290. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.017" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.017</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.017" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.017</a></pub-id>
Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.). (1994). <em>Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture</em>. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511752902" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1017/CBO9780511752902</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (1993). Young children’s understanding of the mind-body distinction. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>64</em>(5), 1534–1549.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/1131551" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/1131551</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Jipson, J. L., & Callanan, M. A. (2003). Mother–child conversation and children’s understanding of biological and nonbiological changes in size. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>74</em>(2), 629–644. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.7402020" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/1467-8624.7402020</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.7402020" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/1467-8624.7402020</a></pub-id>
Leddon, E.M., Waxman, S.R., Medin, (2011). What does it mean to ‘live’ and ‘die’? A cross-linguistic analysis of parent-child conversations in English and Indonesian. <em>British Journal of Developmental Psychology</em>, <em>29</em>(3): 375–395. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1348/026151010X490858" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1348/026151010X490858</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1348/026151010X490858" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1348/026151010X490858</a></pub-id>
Medin, D. L., & Atran, S. (2004). The native mind: biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures. <em>Psychological Review</em>, <em>111</em>(4), 960-983. doi :<a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.960" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.960</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.960" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.960</a></pub-id>
Medin, D. L., Lynch, E. B., Coley, J. D., & Atran, S. (1997). Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: Do all roads lead to Rome? <em>Cognitive Psychology</em>, <em>32</em>(1), 49–96. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1997.0645" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1006/cogp.1997.0645</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1997.0645" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1006/cogp.1997.0645</a></pub-id>
Medin, D. L., Waxman, S., Woodring, J., & Washinawatok, K. (2010). Human-centeredness is not a universal feature of young children’s reasoning: Culture and experience matter when reasoning about biological entities. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>25</em>(3), 197–207. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.02.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.02.001</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.02.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.02.001</a></pub-id>
Namy, L. L., & Gentner, D. (2002). Making a silk purse out of sow’s ears: Young children’s use of comparison in category learning. <em>Journal of Experimental Psychology: General</em>, <em>131</em>, 5–15. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.5" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.5</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.5" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.5</a></pub-id>
National Research Council. (2009). <em>Learning science in informal environments: People, places, and pursuits</em>. Washington DC: National Academies Press.
Opfer, J. E., & Siegler, R. S. (2004). Revisiting preschoolers’ living things concept: A microgenetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology. <em>Cognitive Psychology</em>, <em>49</em>(4), 301–332. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.01.002" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.01.002</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.01.002" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.01.002</a></pub-id>
Palmquist, S., & Crowley, K. (2007). From teachers to testers: How parents talk to novice and expert children in a natural history museum. <em>Science Education</em>, <em>91</em>(5), 783–804. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20215" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1002/sce.20215</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20215" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1002/sce.20215</a></pub-id>
Poziomek, U., Tarłowski, A., Marszał, D., & Ostrowska, E. B. (2015). Edukacja przyrodnicza małych dzieci–opis przyrody czy jej badanie? <em>Edukacja Biologiczna i Środowiskowa</em>, <em>4</em>, 48–61.
Rigney, J. C., & Callanan, M. A. (2011). Patterns in parent–child conversations about animals at a marine science center. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>26</em>(2), 155–171. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.12.002" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.12.002</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.12.002" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.12.002</a></pub-id>
Rosengren, K. S., Gelman, S. A., Kalish, C. W., & McCormick, M. (1991). As time goes by: Children’s early understanding of growth in animals. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>62</em>(6), 1302–1320. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01607.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01607.x</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01607.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01607.x</a></pub-id>
Ross, N., Medin, D., Coley, J. D., & Atran, S. (2003). Cultural and experiential differences in the development of folkbiological induction. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>18</em>(1), 25–47. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00142-9" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00142-9</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00142-9" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00142-9</a></pub-id>
Rybska, E. (2017). <em>Przyroda w osobistych koncepcjach dziecięcych – implikacje dla jej nauczania z wykorzystaniem rysunku</em>. Poznań: Wydawnictwo Kontekst.
Shafto, P., & Coley, J. D. (2003). Development of categorization and reasoning in the natural world: Novices to experts, naive similarity to ecological knowledge. J<em>ournal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition</em>, <em>29</em>(4), 641-649. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.641" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.641</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.641" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.641</a></pub-id>
Shafto, P., Coley, J. D., & Vitkin, A. (2007). Availability in category-based induction. In: A. Feeney and E. Heit (Eds.) <em>Inductive reasoning: Experimental, developmental, and computational approaches</em> (pp 114–136), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511619304" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1017/CBO9780511619304</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511619304" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1017/CBO9780511619304</a></pub-id>
Skura, M. & Lisicki, M. (2012). <em>Na progu. Ile w dziecku ucznia, a w nauczycielu mistrza? O co chodzi w pierwszej klasie?</em> Warszawa: Ośrodek Rozwoju Edukacji
Solomon, G. E., Johnson, S. C., Zaitchik, D., & Carey, S. (1996). Like father, like son: Young children’s understanding of how and why offspring resemble their parents. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>67</em>(1), 151–171. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01726.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01726.x</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01726.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01726.x</a></pub-id>
Springer, K., & Keil, F. C. (1989). On the development of biologically specific beliefs: The case of inheritance. <em>Child Development</em>, 637–648. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/1130729" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/1130729</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/1130729" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/1130729</a></pub-id>
Tarłowski, A. (2006). If it’s an animal it has axons: Experience and culture in preschool children’s reasoning about animates. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>21</em>(3), 249–265. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.02.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.02.001</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.02.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.02.001</a></pub-id>
Tarłowski, A. (2017). The reliance on inclusive living thing in inductive inference among 5-year-olds: the role of access to nature and the size of receptive vocabulary. <em>Psychology of Language and Communication</em>, <em>21</em>(1), 109–132. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1515/plc-2017-0006" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1515/plc-2017-0006</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1515/plc-2017-0006" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1515/plc-2017-0006</a></pub-id>
Valle, A., & Callanan, M. A. (2006). Similarity comparisons and relational analogies in parent-child conversations about science topics. <em>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly</em>, <em>52</em>(1), 96–124. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="https://www.jstor.org/stable/23096069">https://www.jstor.org/stable/23096069</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2006.0009" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1353/mpq.2006.0009</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Waxman, S., Medin, D., & Ross, N. (2007). Folkbiological reasoning from a cross-cultural developmental perspective: Early essentialist notions are shaped by cultural beliefs. <em>Developmental Psychology</em>, <em>43</em>(2), 294-308. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.294" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.294</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.294" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.294</a></pub-id>
Waxman, S. R., Herrmann, P., Woodring, J., & Medin, D. L. (2014). Humans (really) are animals: Picture-book reading influences 5-year-old urban children’s construal of the relation between humans and non-human animals. <em>Frontiers in Psychology</em>, <em>5</em>:172. doi: <a href="https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00172" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00172</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00172" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00172</a></pub-id>