Apperly, I. (2013). Can theory of mind grow up? Mindreading in adults, and its implications for the development and neuroscience of mindreading. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Lombardo, M. (Eds.) <em>Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental social neuroscience</em> (pp. 72–92). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199692972.003.0005" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199692972.003.0005</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Apperly, I. A. & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? <em>Psychological review</em>, <em>116</em>(4), 953.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016923" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/a0016923</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Baillargeon, R., Buttelmann, D., & Southgate, V. (2018). Invited commentary: Interpreting failed replications of early false-belief findings: Methodological and theoretical considerations. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>46</em>, 112–124.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.06.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.06.001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Baker, S. T., Leslie, A. M., Gallistel, C. R., & Hood, B. M. (2016). Bayesian change-point analysis reveals developmental change in a classic theory of mind task. <em>Cognitive psychology, 91</em>, 124–149.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.08.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.08.001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Bardi, L., Desmet, C., Nijhof, A., Wiersema, J. R., & Brass, M. (2016). Brain activation for spontaneous and explicit false belief tasks overlaps: new fMRI evidence on belief processing and violation of expectation. <em>Social cognitive and affective neuroscience</em>, <em>12</em>(3), 391–400.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw143" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/scan/nsw143</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Bardi, L., Six, P., & Brass, M. (2017). Repetitive TMS of the temporo-parietal junction disrupts participant’s expectations in a spontaneous Theory of Mind task. <em>Social cognitive and affective neuroscience</em>, <em>12</em>(11), 1775–1782.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx109" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/scan/nsx109</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. <em>Journal of child Psychology and Psychiatry</em>, <em>30</em>(2), 285–297.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>21</em>(1), 37–46.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Benson, J. E., Sabbagh, M. A., Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers’ improvement from theory-of-mind training. <em>Developmental Psychology</em>, <em>49</em>(9), 1615.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031056" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/a0031056</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Birch, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2003). Children are cursed: An asymmetric bias in mental-state attribution. <em>Psychological Science</em>, <em>14</em>(3), 283–286.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.03436" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/1467-9280.03436</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Birch, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false-beliefs. <em>Psychological Science</em>, <em>18</em>(5), 382–386.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01909.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01909.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Biro, S., Csibra, G., Koos, O, & Gergely, G. (1998). Understanding rational action in infancy. <em>Psychology of Language and Communication</em>, <em>1</em>(2), 29–38. Bloom, P., & German, T. P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false-belief task as a test of theory of mind. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>77</em>(1), B25–B31.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00096-2" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00096-2</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Buttelmann, D., Buttelmann, F., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Great apes distinguish true from false-beliefs in an interactive helping task. <em>PLoS one</em>, <em>12</em>(4), e0173793.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173793" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1371/journal.pone.0173793</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false-belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>112</em>(2), 337–342.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Buttelmann, D., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Eighteen-month-olds understand false-beliefs in an unexpected-contents task. <em>Journal of Experimental Child Psychology</em>, <em>119</em>, 120–126.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.002" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.002</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Buttelmann, F., Suhrke, J., & Buttelmann, D. (2015). What you get is what you believe: Eighteen-month-olds demonstrate belief understanding in an unexpected-identity task. <em>Journal of Experimental Child Psychology</em>, <em>131</em>, 94–103.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.009" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.009</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Carter, C. S., & Van Veen, V. (2007). Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: an update of theory and data. <em>Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience</em>, <em>7</em>(4), 367-379.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.4.367" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3758/CABN.7.4.367</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>9</em>(4), 377–395.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (2000). <em>Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false. Children’s Reasoning and the Mind</em> (Eds.), P. Mitchell and K. Riggs (Hove: Psychology Press), 191–228.
de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>17</em>(1), 1037–1060.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false-belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>85</em>(5), 1777–1794.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/cdev.12237</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2018). What do monkeys know about others’ knowledge?. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>170</em>, 201–208.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.10.004" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cognition.2017.10.004</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Farrar, M. J., Benigno, J. P., Tompkins, V., & Gage, N. A. (2017). Are there different pathways to explicit false-belief understanding? General language and complementation in typical and atypical children. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>43</em>, 49–66.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.005" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.005</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Flavell, J. H. (1993). The development of children’s understanding of false-belief and the appearance-reality distinction. <em>International Journal of Psychology</em>, <em>28</em>(5), 595–604.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/00207599308246944" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/00207599308246944</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>57</em>(2), 109–128.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00692-R" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(95)00692-R</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. <em>Trends in Cognitive Sciences</em>, <em>7</em>(2), 77–83.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Gallagher, S., & Povinelli, D. J. (2012). Enactive and behavioral abstraction accounts of social understanding in chimpanzees, infants, and adults. <em>Review of Philosophy and Psychology</em>, <em>3</em>(1), 145–169.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-012-0093-4" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1007/s13164-012-0093-4</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Grosse Wiesmann, C., Friederici, A. D., Disla, D., Steinbeis, N., & Singer, T. (2018). Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds’ but not 2-and 3-year-olds’ false-belief-related action anticipation. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>46</em>, 58–68.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Grosse Wiesmann, C., Friederici, A. D., Singer, T., & Steinbeis, N. (2017). Implicit and explicit false-belief development in preschool children. <em>Developmental Science</em>, <em>20</em>(5).<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12445" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/desc.12445</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Grosse Wiesmann, C., Schreiber, J., Singer, T., Steinbeis, N., & Friederici, A. D. (2017). White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of theory of mind in early childhood. <em>Nature Communications</em>, <em>8</em>, 14692.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14692" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1038/ncomms14692</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Gut, A., Haman, M. & Gorbaniuk, O. (under review) The development of understanding opacity in preschoolers: A transition from a coarse- to fine-grained understanding of beliefs.
Gweon, H., Dodell-Feder, D., Bedny, M., & Saxe, R. (2012). Theory of mind performance in children correlates with functional specialization of a brain region for thinking about thoughts. <em>Child development</em>, <em>83</em>(6), 1853–1868.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01829.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01829.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Helming, K. A., Strickland, B., & Jacob, P. (2014). Making sense of early false-belief understanding. <em>Trends in Cognitive Sciences</em>, <em>18</em>(4), 167–170.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.005" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.005</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Helming, K. A., Strickland, B., & Jacob, P. (2016). Solving the puzzle about early belief-ascription. <em>Mind & Language</em>, <em>31</em>(4), 438–469.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12114" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/mila.12114</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T., & Bailey, P. E. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. <em>Psychology and Aging</em>, <em>28</em>(3), 826.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030677" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/a0030677</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Heyes, C. (2014b). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind. <em>Perspectives on Psychological Science</em>, <em>9</em>(2), 131–143.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613518076" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1177/1745691613518076</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Hoehl, S., Wiese, L., & Striano, T. (2008). Young infants’ neural processing of objects is affected by eye gaze direction and emotional expression. <em>PLoS One</em>, <em>3</em>(6), e2389.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0002389" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1371/journal.pone.0002389</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. <em>Psychological Science</em>, <em>9</em>(2), 131–134.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00024" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/1467-9280.00024</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. <em>Social Development</em>, <em>13</em>(4), 590–619.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00285.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00285.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Hyde, D. C., Simon, C. E., Ting, F., & Nikolaeva, J. I. (2018). Functional Organization of the Temporal–Parietal Junction for Theory of Mind in Preverbal Infants: A Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Study. <em>Journal of Neuroscience</em>, <em>38</em>(18), 4264–4274.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0264-17.2018" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0264-17.2018</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Jacoby, N., Bruneau, E., Koster-Hale, J., & Saxe, R. (2016). Localizing pain matrix and theory of mind networks with both verbal and non-verbal stimuli. <em>NeuroImage</em>, <em>126</em>, 39–48.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.025" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.025</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (2000). Theory of mind and social behavior: Causal models tested in a longitudinal study. <em>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982-)</em>, 203–220.
Kampis, D., Fogd, D., & Kovács, Á. M. (2017). Nonverbal components of Theory of Mind in typical and atypical development. <em>Infant Behavior and Development</em>, <em>48</em>, 54–62.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Kovács, Á. M., Kühn, S., Gergely, G., Csibra, G., & Brass, M. (2014). Are all beliefs equal? Implicit belief attributions recruiting core brain regions of theory of mind. <em>PloS one</em>, <em>9</em>(9), e106558.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106558" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1371/journal.pone.0106558</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. <em>Science</em>, <em>330</em>(6012), 1830–1834.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1190792" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1126/science.1190792</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
El Kaddouri, R., Bardi, L., De Bremaeker, D., Brass, M., & Wiersema, J. R. (2019). Measuring spontaneous mentalizing with a ball detection task: putting the attention-check hypothesis by Phillips and colleagues (2015) to the test. <em>Psychological Research</em>, 1–9.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01181-7" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1007/s00426-019-01181-7</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false-beliefs. <em>Science</em>, <em>354</em>(6308), 110–114.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf8110" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1126/science.aaf8110</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes’ performance in a false-belief task inanimate control. <em>Communicative & Integrative Biology</em>, <em>10</em>(4), e1343771.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/19420889.2017.1343771" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/19420889.2017.1343771</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Kuhn, D. (2000). Theory of mind, metacognition, and reasoning: A life-span perspective. In P. Mitchell & K. J. Riggs (Eds.) <em>Children’s reasoning and the mind</em> (pp. 301–326). Hove: Psychology Press.
Kulke, L., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Implicit theory of mind – An overview of current replications and non-replications. <em>Data in Brief</em>, <em>16</em>, 101–104.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Kulke, L., Reiß, M., Krist, H., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). How robust are anticipatory looking measures of theory of mind? Replication attempts across the life span. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>46</em>, 97–111.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.09.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.09.001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Kulke, L., von Duhn, B., Schneider, D., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Is implicit theory of mind a real and robust phenomenon? Results from a systematic replication study. <em>Psychological Science</em>, 0956797617747090.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617747090" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1177/0956797617747090</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Lackner, C., Sabbagh, M. A., Hallinan, E., Liu, X., & Holden, J. J. (2012). Dopamine receptor D4 gene variation predicts preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. <em>Developmental Science</em>, <em>15</em>(2), 272–280.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01124.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01124.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Lackner, C. L., Bowman, L. C., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2010). Dopaminergic functioning and preschoolers’ theory of mind. <em>Neuropsychologia</em>, <em>48</em>(6), 1767–1774.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.027" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.027</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Leslie, A. M., Xu, F., Tremoulet, P. D., & Scholl, B. J. (1998). Indexing and the object concept: developingwhat’andwhere’systems. <em>Trends in Cognitive Sciences</em>, <em>2</em>(1), 10–18.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01113-3" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01113-3</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Low, J., Apperly, I. A., Butterfill, S. A., & Rakoczy, H. (2016). Cognitive architecture of reasoning in children and adults: A primer on the two-systems account. <em>Child Development Perspectives</em>, <em>10</em>(3), 184-189.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12183" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/cdep.12183</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Mars, R. B., Neubert, F. X., Noonan, M. P., Sallet, J., Toni, I., & Rushworth, M. F. (2012). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. <em>Frontiers in Human Neuroscience</em>, <em>6</em>, 189.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Meert, G., Wang, J., & Samson, D. (2017). Efficient belief tracking in adults: The role of task instruction, low-level associative processes and dispositional social functioning. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>168</em>, 91–98.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.012" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.012</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Meltzoff, A. N. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. <em>Understanding other minds: Perspectives from Autism</em>, 335–366.
Moran, J. M., Young, L. L., Saxe, R., Lee, S. M., O’Young, D., Mavros, P. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. <em>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences</em>, <em>108</em>(7), 2688–2692.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011734108" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1073/pnas.1011734108</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Naughtin, C. K., Horne, K., Schneider, D., Venini, D., York, A., & Dux, P. E. (2017). Do implicit and explicit belief processing share neural substrates? <em>Human Brain Mapping</em>, <em>38</em>(9), 4760–4772.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23700" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1002/hbm.23700</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Nelson, K., Plesa, D., & Henseler, S. (1998). Children’s theory of mind: An experiential interpretation. <em>Human Development</em>, <em>41</em>(1), 7–29.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1159/000022565" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1159/000022565</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Nijhof, A. D., Brass, M., Bardi, L., & Wiersema, J. R. (2016). Measuring mentalizing ability: A within-subject comparison between an explicit and implicit version of a ball detection task. <em>PloS one</em>, <em>11</em>(10): e0164373. doi:<a href="https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164373" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1371/journal.pone.0164373</a><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164373" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1371/journal.pone.0164373</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Nijhof, A. D., Bardi, L., Brass, M., & Wiersema, J. R. (2018). Brain activity for spontaneous and explicit mentalizing in adults with autism spectrum disorder: An fMRI study. <em>NeuroImage: Clinical</em>, <em>18</em>, 475–484.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.016" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.016</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Oktay-Gür, N., Schulz, A., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Children exhibit different performance patterns in explicit and implicit theory of mind tasks. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>173</em>, 60–74.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., Moore, C., & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Peer social skills and theory of mind in children with autism, deafness, or typical development. <em>Developmental Psychology</em>, <em>52</em>(1), 46.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039833" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/a0039833</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V. P., & Paynter, J. (2007). Social maturity and theory of mind in typically developing children and those on the autism spectrum. <em>Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry</em>, <em>48</em>(12), 1243–1250.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01810.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01810.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Phillips, J., Ong, D. C., Surtees, A. D., Xin, Y., Williams, S., Saxe, R., & Frank, M. C. (2015). A second look at automatic theory of mind: Reconsidering Kovács, Téglás, and Endress (2010). <em>Psychological Science</em>, <em>26</em>(9), 1353–1367.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614558717" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1177/0956797614558717</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Poulin-Dubois, D., & Yott, J. (2018). Probing the depth of infants’ theory of mind: Disunity in performance across paradigms. <em>Developmental Science</em>, <em>21</em>(4), e12600.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12600" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/desc.12600</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Premack, D. (1990). The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>36</em>(1), 1–16.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90051-K" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(90)90051-K</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? <em>Behavioral and Brain Sciences</em>, <em>1</em>(4), 515–526.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1017/S0140525X00076512</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Priewasser, B., Rafetseder, E., Gargitter, C., & Perner, J. (2017). Helping as an early indicator of a theory of mind: Mentalism or teleology?. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>46</em>, 69–78. doi:<a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002</a><pub-id pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002</a></pub-id>
Rabinowitz, N. C., Perbet, F., Song, H. F., Zhang, C., Eslami, S. M., & Botvinick, M. (2018). <em>Machine theory of mind</em>. arXiv preprint arXiv:1802.07740.
Richardson, H., Lisandrelli, G., Riobueno-Naylor, A., & Saxe, R. (2018). Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years. <em>Nature Communications</em>, <em>9</em>(1), 1027.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-03399-2" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1038/s41467-018-03399-2</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Rubio-Fernández, P., & Geurts, B. (2013). How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday. <em>Psychological Science</em>, <em>24</em>(1), 27–33.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612447819" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1177/0956797612447819</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Rubio-Fernández, P., Jara-Ettinger, J., & Gibson, E. (2017). Can processing demands explain toddlers’ performance in false-belief tasks?. <em>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences</em>, <em>114</em>(19), E3750–E3750.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1701286114" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1073/pnas.1701286114</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Sabbagh, M. A., Bowman, L. C., Evraire, L. E., & Ito, J. (2009). Neurodevelopmental correlates of theory of mind in preschool children. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>80</em>(4), 1147–1162.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01322.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01322.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Samson, D., Apperly, I., Braithwaite, J., Andrews, B., & Bodley Scott, S. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. <em>Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance</em>, <em>36</em>, 1255–1266.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018729" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/a0018729</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schaafsma, S. M., Pfaff, D. W., Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2015). Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind. <em>Trends in Cognitive Sciences</em>, <em>19</em>(2), 65–72.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.007" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.007</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Scheeren, A. M., de Rosnay, M., Koot, H. M., & Begeer, S. (2013). Rethinking theory of mind in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. <em>Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry</em>, <em>54</em>(6), 628–635.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12007" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/jcpp.12007</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>78</em>(2), 376–396.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schneider, D., Lam, R., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2012). Cognitive load disrupts implicit theory-of-mind processing. <em>Psychological Science</em>, <em>23</em>(8), 842–847.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612439070" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1177/0956797612439070</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Becker, S. I., & Dux, P. E. (2014). Implicit false-belief processing in the human brain. <em>NeuroImage</em>, <em>101</em>, 268–275.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.014" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.014</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schneider, D., Nott, Z. E., & Dux, P. E. (2014). Task instructions and implicit theory of mind. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>133</em>, 43–47.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.016" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.016</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. <em>Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews</em>, <em>42</em>, 9–34.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schurz, M., Tholen, M. G., Perner, J., Mars, R. B., & Sallet, J. (2017). Specifying the brain anatomy underlying temporo-parietal junction activations for theory of mind: A review using probabilistic atlases from different imaging modalities. <em>Human Brain Mapping</em>, <em>38</em>(9), 4788–4805.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23675" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1002/hbm.23675</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schuwerk, T., Vuori, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). Implicit and explicit theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders: The impact of experience. <em>Autism, 19</em>(4), 459–468.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361314526004" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1177/1362361314526004</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Scott, R. M. (2014). Post hoc versus predictive accounts of children’s theory of mind: A reply to Ruffman. <em>Developmental Review</em>, <em>34</em>(3), 300–304.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>80</em>(4), 1172–1196.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01324.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01324.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Early false-belief understanding. <em>Trends in Cognitive Sciences</em>, 21(4), 237–249.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., & Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. <em>Developmental Psychology</em>, <em>47</em>(5), 1239.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023899" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/a0023899</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Slaughter, V. (2015). Theory of mind in infants and young children: a review. <em>Australian Psychologist</em>, <em>50</em>(3), 169–172.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/ap.12080" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/ap.12080</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Sodian, B. (2016). Is false-belief understanding continuous from infancy to preschool age? In D. Barner & A. S. Baron (Eds.), <em>Core knowledge and conceptual change</em> (p. 301), Oxford: Oxford University Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190467630.003.0017" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190467630.003.0017</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Sommerville, J. A., Bernstein, D. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2013). Measuring beliefs in centimeters: Private knowledge biases preschoolers’ and adults’ representation of others’ beliefs. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>84</em>(6), 1846–1854.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12110" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/cdev.12110</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Southgate, V. (2018). The puzzle of early mentalizing: A unitary theory of infants’ successes and preschoolers’ failures. doi:<a href="https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/Y2GHQ" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.17605/OSF.IO/Y2GHQ</a>
Spreng, R. N., & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. <em>Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience</em>, <em>22</em>(6), 1112–1123.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21282" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1162/jocn.2009.21282</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Thoermer, C., Sodian, B., Vuori, M., Perst, H., & Kristen, S. (2012). Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false-belief from infancy to preschool age. <em>British Journal of Developmental Psychology</em>, <em>30</em>(1), 172–187.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Tomasello, M. (2018). How children come to understand false-beliefs: A shared intentionality account. <em>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences</em>, <em>115</em>(34), 8491–8498.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804761115" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1073/pnas.1804761115</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Wang, L., & Leslie, A. M. (2016). Is Implicit Theory of Mind the ‘Real Deal’? The Own-Belief/True-Belief Default in Adults and Young Preschoolers. <em>Mind & Language</em>, <em>31</em>(2), 147–176.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12099" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/mila.12099</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Wellman, H. M. (2014). <em>Making minds: How theory of mind develops</em>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199334919.001.0001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199334919.001.0001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Wellman, H. M. (2017). The development of theory of mind: Historical reflections. <em>Child Development Perspectives</em>, <em>11</em>(3), 207–214.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12236" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/cdep.12236</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>75</em>(2), 523–541.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>35</em>, 245–275.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90024-E" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(90)90024-E</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale: Longitudinal perspectives. <em>Child Development</em>, <em>82</em>(3), 780–792.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01583.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01583.x</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>13</em>(1), 103–128.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Yott, J., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2016). Are infants’ theory-of-mind abilities well integrated? Implicit understanding of intentions, desires, and beliefs. <em>Journal of Cognition and Development</em>, <em>17</em>(5), 683–698.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2015.1086771" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/15248372.2015.1086771</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Zaitchik, D. (1990). When representations conflict with reality: The preschooler’s problem with false-beliefs and “false” photographs. <em>Cognition</em>, <em>35</em>(1), 41–68.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90036-J" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/0010-0277(90)90036-J</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M., & Kesek, A. (2008). The development of executive function in childhood. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), <em>Developmental cognitive neuroscience. Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience</em> (pp. 553–574). Cambridge, MA, US: MIT Pres