Aksu-Koç, A., & Tekdemir, G. (2004). Interplay between narrativity and mindreading: A comparison between Turkish and English. In S. Strömqvist & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspective (pp. 307–327). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Astington, J. W. (2001). The future of theory-of-mind research: Understanding motivational states, the role of language, and real-world consequences. Child Development, 72, 685–687.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00305" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/1467-8624.00305</a>
Astington, J. W. (2006). The developmental interdependence of theory of mind and language. In S. C. Levinson, & N. J. Enfield (Eds.), The roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction (pp. 179–206). Oxford: Berg.<a href="https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003135517-9" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.4324/9781003135517-9</a>
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H.M. (1995). Children's talk about the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195080056.001.0001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/oso/9780195080056.001.0001</a>
Berman, R. A. (2004). The role of context in developing narrative abilities. In S. Strömqvist & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives (pp. 261–280). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Berman, R. A. (2009). Trends in research on narrative development. In S. Foster-Cohen (Ed.), Language acquisition (pp. 294–318). Basingstoke: Palgrave-Macmillan.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230240780_13" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1057/9780230240780_13</a>
Bokus, B. (1996). Narrative space structuring at the preschool age: Findings on monologic and dialogic discourse. In C. E. Johnson & J. H. V. Gilbert (Eds.), Children’s language, Vol. 9 (pp. 197–207). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bokus, B. (1998). Action and its representation in the minds of story characters: Findings from children’s discourse. Psychology of Language and Communication, 2, 63–77.
Bokus, B. (2013). Mind-reading in children’s narration: Mental states ascribed to narrative line and narrative field subjects. In G. Shugar Wales, B. Bokus & J. Smogorzewska (Eds.), From reference situation to narrative text, vol. 21 (pp. 77–111). Piaseczno, Poland: Studio Lexem. ISBN 9788393625857.
Chandler, M. (2001). Perspective taking in the aftermath of theory-theory and the collapse of the social role-taking literature. In A. Tryphon & J. Vonèche (Eds.), Working with Piaget: In memoriam–Bärbel Inhelder (pp. 39–63). Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press.
Dunn, J. (1991). Understanding others: evidence from naturalistic studies of children. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind (pp. 51–61) Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversation about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 132–139.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.132" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.132</a>
Eaton, J. H., Collis, G. N., & Lewis, V. A. (1999). Evaluative explanations in children’s narratives of a video sequence without dialogue. Journal of Child Language, 26, 699–720.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000999003967" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1017/S0305000999003967</a>
Eisenberg, A. R (1985). Learning to describe past experiences in conversation. Discourse Processes, 8, 177–204.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/01638538509544613" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/01638538509544613</a>
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research on Child Development, 51(1, Serial No. 212).<a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/1165866" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/1165866</a>
Hausendorf, H., & Quasthoff, U.M. (1992). Children’s storytelling in adult-child interaction: three dimensions in narrative development. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 2, 293–306.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/jnlh.2.3.07chi" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/jnlh.2.3.07chi</a>
Hickmann, M. (1995). Discourse organization and reference. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Handbook of Child Language (pp. 194–218). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Melot, A. M. (1999). Développement cognitif et métacognitif : panorama d'un nouveau courant. Enfance, 3, 205–214.<a href="https://doi.org/10.3406/enfan.1999.3144" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3406/enfan.1999.3144</a>
Miller, P. J., & Sperry, L. L. (1988). Early talk about the past: The origins of conversational stories of personal experience. Journal of Child Language, 15, 293–315.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012381" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1017/S0305000900012381</a>
Nelson, K. (1999). Event representations, narrative development and internal working models. Attachment and Human Development, 1, 239–252.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/14616739900134131" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/14616739900134131</a>
Nelson, K., & Khan, K. (2019). New frontiers in facilitating narrative skills in children and adolescents: A dynamic systems account incorporating eight narrative developmental stages. In E. Veneziano & A. Nicolopoulou (Eds), Narratives, literacy and other skills: Studies in intervention (pp. 173-200). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/sin.25.09nel" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/sin.25.09nel</a>
O'Neill, D. K. (1996). Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659–677.<a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/1131839" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/1131839</a>
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1987.tb01048.x" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/j.2044-835X.1987.tb01048.x</a>
Piaget, J. (1941). Le mécanisme du développement mental et les lois du groupement des opérations : esquisse d'une théorie opératoire de l'intelligence. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 215–285.
Repacholi B. M., & Gopnik A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12–21.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12</a>
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: the emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children's language. Vol. 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Veneziano, E. (2001). Interactional processes in the origins of the explaining capacity. In K. Nelson, A. Aksu-Koc, and C. Johnson (Eds.), Children’s language: Developing narrative and discourse competence (pp. 113–141). Mahwah, N.J.: L. Erlbaum.
Veneziano, E. (2009). Language and internal states: A long developmental history at different levels of functioning. RIPLA Rivista di Psicolinguistica applicata /Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, IX, 9–27.
Veneziano, E. (2010). Peut-on aider l’enfant à mieux raconter? Les effets de différentes méthodes d’intervention. In H. Makdissi, A. Boisclair & P. Sirois (Eds.), La littératie au préscolaire: une fenêtre ouverte sur la scolarisation (pp. 107–144). Québec: Presses Universitaires du Quebec.<a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctv18ph0gv.8" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/j.ctv18ph0gv.8</a>
Veneziano, E. (2016). The development of narrative discourse in French by 5 to 10 year old children: Some insights from a conversational interaction method. In J. Perera, M. Aparici, E. Rosado, & N. Salas (Eds.), Written and spoken language development across the lifespan: Essays in honour of Liliana Tolchinsky (pp. 141–160). New York, NY: Springer Verlag.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-21136-7_10" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1007/978-3-319-21136-7_10</a>
Veneziano, E. (2017). Talking about the nonliteral: Internal states and explanations in child-constructed narratives. Psychology of Language and Communication, 21(1), 134–151. DOI: <a href="https://doi.org/10.1515/plc-2017-000710.1515/plc-2017-0007" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1515/plc-2017-000710.1515/plc-2017-0007</a>
Veneziano, E., Albert, L., & Martin, S. (2009). Learning to tell a story of false belief: A study of French-speaking children. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, N. Budwig, S. Ervin-Tripp, K Nakamura, & S. Özçaliskan (Eds), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Research in the tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin (pp. 277–289). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Veneziano, E., & Hudelot, C. (2006). Etats internes, fausse croyance et explications dans les récits: effets de l’étayage chez les enfants de 4 à 12 ans. Langage et l'Homme, 41(2), 119–140.
Veneziano, E., & Hudelot, C. (2009). Explaining events in narratives: the impact of scaffolding in 4 to 12 year-old children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 13(1), 3–20.<a href="https://doi.org/10.2478/v10057-009-0001-X" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2478/v10057-009-0001-X</a>
Veneziano, E., Hudelot, C., LeNormand, M. T., Plumet, M. H., & Elie, J. (forthcoming). Improving narrative skills in 5- to 8-year-old French-speaking children: Immediate and delayed effects of a short conversational intervention.
Veneziano, E., & Plumet, M. H. (2019). Promoting narratives through a short conversational intervention in typically developing and high-functioning children with ASD. In E. Veneziano & A. Nicolopoulou (Eds), Narratives, literacy and other skills: Studies in intervention (pp.285-312). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/sin.25.14ven" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/sin.25.14ven</a>
Veneziano, E., Plumet, M. H., Angeard, N., & Moutier, S. (2012). Attribution of false belief in French children: Décalages between tasks and cultural differences. 42nd Annual Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Toronto, Canada.
Veneziano, E., & Sinclair, H. (1995). Functional changes in early child language: The appearance of references to the past and of explanations. Journal of Child Language, 22, 557–581.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900009958" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1017/S0305000900009958</a>
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–12810.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5