Have a personal or library account? Click to login
The Interplay of Syntactic and Lexical Salience and its Effect on Default Figurative Responses Cover

The Interplay of Syntactic and Lexical Salience and its Effect on Default Figurative Responses

By:
Open Access
|Mar 2020

References

  1. Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639</a>
  2. Bergen, B. K. (2012). Louder than words: The new science of how the mind makes meaning. New York: Basic Books.
  3. Chafe, W. (1994). Discourse, consciousness and time: The flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
  4. Charteris-Black, J. (2012). Shattering the bell jar: Metaphor, gender and depression. Metaphor and symbol, 27(3), 199–216.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2012.665796" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/10926488.2012.665796</a>
  5. Coulson, S. (2008). Metaphor comprehension and the brain. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 177–194). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  6. Coulson, S., & van Petten, C. (2002). Conceptual integration and metaphor: an event-related potential study. Memory and Cognition, 30(6), 958–968.<a href="https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195780" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3758/BF03195780</a>
  7. Coulson, S., & Lai, V. T. (2015). The metaphorical brain. Lausanne: Frontiers Media.
  8. Divjak, D. (2015). Exploring the grammar of perception. A case study using data from Russian. Functions of Language, 22(1), 44–68.
  9. Faucett, J. M., Risco, E. F., & Kingstone, A. (2015). The handbook of attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<a href="https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/10033.001.0001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.7551/mitpress/10033.001.0001</a>
  10. Gentner, D., Bowdle, B. F., Wolff, P., & Boronat, C. (2001). Metaphor is like analogy. In D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak & B. N. Kokinov (Eds.), The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 199–253). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  11. Gibbs, R. W. (1992). Categorization and metaphor understanding. Psychological Review, 99, 572–577.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.572" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.572</a>
  12. Gibbs, R. W., & Matlock, T. (2008). Metaphor, imagination, and simulation: Psycholinguistic evidence. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 161–176). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  13. Giora, R. (1999). On the priority of salient meanings: Studies of literal and figurative language. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(7), 919–929.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(98)00100-3" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0378-2166(98)00100-3</a>
  14. Giora, R. (2003). On our mind: Salience, context, and figurative language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  15. Giora, R., & Fein, O. (1999). Irony: Context and salience. Metaphor and Symbol, 14(4), 241–257.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327868MS1404_1" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1207/S15327868MS1404_1</a>
  16. Giora, R., Givoni, S., & Fein, O. (2015). Defaultness reigns: The case of sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 30(4), 290–313.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2015.1074804" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1080/10926488.2015.1074804</a>
  17. Giora, R., Jaffe, I., Becker, I., & Fein, O. (2018). Strongly mitigating a highly positive concept: The case of default sarcastic interpretations. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 6(1), 19–47.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.00002.gio" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/rcl.00002.gio</a>
  18. Givón, T. (1987). Beyond foreground and background. In R. S. Tomlin (Ed.), Coherence and grounding in discourse (pp. 175–168). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  19. Glucksberg, S., Gildea, P., & Bookin, H. B. (1982). On understanding non-literal speech: Can people ignore metaphors? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 85–96.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90467-4" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90467-4</a>
  20. Glucksberg, S., & Keysar, B. (1990). Understanding metaphorical comparisons: Beyond similarity. Psychological Review, 97(1), 3–18.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.3" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.3</a>
  21. Gold, R., & Faust, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dysfunction and metaphor comprehension in young adults with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(7), 800–811.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0930-1" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1007/s10803-009-0930-1</a>
  22. Golubkova, E. E. (2014). Ispolzovaniye lingvisticheskih korpusov pri reshenii semanticheskikh problem. In V. I. Zabotkina & E. E. Golubkova (Eds.), Metody kognitivnogo analiza semantiki slova: Komputerno-korpusnyi podkhod (pp. 34–75). Moscow: Yazyki Slavianskoi Kultury. (In Russ.).
  23. Gries, S. T., & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.) (2006). Corpora in cognitive linguistics: Corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110197709" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1515/9783110197709</a>
  24. Hetmański, M. (2015). Metaphoric confinement of information. Studies in Logic, Grammar, and Rhetoric, 40(53), 161–178.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1515/slgr-2015-0009" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1515/slgr-2015-0009</a>
  25. Iakimova, G., Passerioux, C., Denhière, G., Laurent, J.-P., Vistoli, D., Vilain, J., & Hardy-Baylé, M.-C. (2010). The influence of idiomatic salience during the comprehension of ambiguous idioms by patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 177(1–2), 46–54.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.02.005" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.psychres.2010.02.005</a>
  26. Iriskhanova, O. (2014). Igry fokusa v yazyke: Semantika, sintaksis i pragmatika defokusirovaniya. Moscow: Yazyki Slavianskoi Kultury. (In Russ.).
  27. Kauschke, C., Mueller, N., Kircher, T., & Nagels, A. (2018). Do patients with depression prefer literal or metaphorical expressions for internal states? Evidence from sentence completion and elicited production. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1326.<a href="https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01326" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01326</a>
  28. Kazmerski, V. A., Blasko, D. G., & Dessalegn, B. G. (2003). ERP and behavioral evidence of individual differences in metaphor comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 31(5), 673–689.<a href="https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196107" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3758/BF03196107</a>
  29. Kiose, M. (2018). Factors of co-referent indirect names interpretation in text: Cognitive analysis – statistics – experiment. Issues in Cognitive Linguistics (Voprosy kognitivnoy lingvistiki), 3, 16–26. (In Russ.).<a href="https://doi.org/10.20916/1812-3228-2018-3-16-26" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.20916/1812-3228-2018-3-16-26</a>
  30. Kintsch, W., & Bowles, A. (2002). Metaphor comprehension: What makes a metaphor difficult to understand? Metaphor and Symbol, 17(4), 249–262.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327868MS1704_1" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1207/S15327868MS1704_1</a>
  31. Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2006). Tracking the mind during reading. The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 12–35.
  32. MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2004). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20, 13–32.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1191/0265659004ct261oa" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1191/0265659004ct261oa</a>
  33. National corpus of the Russian language. Retrieved from http://www.ruscorpora.ru/
  34. Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 383–399.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X26732" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1348/026151005X26732</a>
  35. Pynte, J., Besson, M., Robichon, F.-H., & Poli, J. (1996). The time-course of metaphor comprehension: an event-related potential study. Brain and Language, 316, 293–316.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1996.0107" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1006/brln.1996.0107</a>
  36. Rapp, A. M., Felsenheimer, A. K., Langohr, K., & Klupp, M. (2017). The comprehension of familiar and novel metaphoric meanings in schizophrenia: A pilot study. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 2251.
  37. Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372</a>
  38. Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  39. Ritchie, D. (2013). Metaphor. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139136822" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1017/CBO9781139136822</a>
  40. Rundblad, G., & Annaz, D. (2010). The atypical development of metaphor and metonymy comprehension in children with autism. Autism, 14, 29–46.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361309340667" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1177/1362361309340667</a>
  41. Semino, E., & Short, M. (2004). Corpus stylistics: Speech, writing and thought presentation in a corpus of English writing. London: Routledge.<a href="https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203494073" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.4324/9780203494073</a>
  42. Schmid, H.-J. (2007). Entrenchment, salience, and basic levels. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 117–138). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  43. Staub, A. (2015). Reading sentences: Syntactic parsing and semantic interpretation. In A. Pollatsek & R. Treiman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of reading (pp. 202–216). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  44. Steen, G., Dorst, A. G., Herrmann, J. B., Kaal, A. A., Krennmayr, T., & Pasma, T. (2010). A method for linguistic metaphor identification: From MIP to MIPVU. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/celcr.14" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/celcr.14</a>
  45. Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. 1. Concept structuring systems. Cambridge, MA and London: MIT Press.
  46. Tomlin, R. S. (1987). Linguistic reflections of cognitive events. In R. S. Tomlin (Ed.), Coherence and grounding in discourse (pp. 455–479). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
  47. Tognini-Bonelli, E. (2001). Corpus linguistics at work. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.<a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/scl.6" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/scl.6</a>
  48. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. In A. Tversky & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 3–28). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  49. Verhagen, A. (2007). Construal and perspectivization. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 48–81). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  50. Vulchanova, M., Saldańa, D., Chahboun, S., & Vulchanov, V. (2015). Figurative language processing in atypical populations: the ASD perspective. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, Article 24.<a href="https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00024" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3389/fnhum.2015.00024</a>
  51. Wårwik, B. (2004). What is foregrounded in narratives? Hypotheses for the cognitive basis of foregrounding. In T. Virtanen (Ed.), Approaches to cognition through text and discourse (pp. 99–122). Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.2478/slgr-2020-0004 | Journal eISSN: 2199-6059 | Journal ISSN: 0860-150X
Language: English
Page range: 69 - 88
Published on: Mar 20, 2020
Published by: University of Bialystok
In partnership with: Paradigm Publishing Services
Publication frequency: 4 times per year
Related subjects:

© 2020 Maria Kiose, published by University of Bialystok
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 License.