Bolinger, D. (1989). <em>Intonation and its uses</em>. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1515/9781503623125" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1515/9781503623125</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Bourdieu, P. (1984). <em>Distinction: A social critique of the judgment of taste</em>. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315775357" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315775357</a>">https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315775357</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315775357" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.4324/9781315775357</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Fischer, R., Karl, J. A., & Fischer, M. V. (2019). Norms across cultures: A cross-cultural meta-analysis of norms effects in the theory of planned behavior. <em>Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology</em>, <em>50</em>(10), 1112–1126. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022119846409" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022119846409</a>">https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022119846409</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022119846409" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1177/0022022119846409</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Fitch, K. L., & Sanders, R. E. (Eds.). (2005). <em>Handbook of language and social interaction</em>. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Geary, D. C. (2010). <em>Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences</em>. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/12072-000" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1037/12072-000</a>">https://doi.org/10.1037/12072-000</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1037/12072-000" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1037/12072-000</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Gelman, S. A. (2010). Generics as a window onto young children’s concepts. In F. J. Pelletier (Ed.), <em>Kinds, things, and stuff: The cognitive side of generics and mass terms</em> (pp. 100–122). New York: Oxford University Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195382891.003.0006" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195382891.003.0006</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Ghazizadeh, A., Griggs, W., & Hikosaka, O. (2016). Ecological origins of object salience: Reward, uncertainty, aversiveness, and novelty. <em>Frontiers in Neuro-science</em>, <em>10</em>, 378.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00378" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3389/fnins.2016.00378</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Glüer, K., & Wikforss, Å. (2018). The normativity of meaning and content. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), <em>Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy</em>. Retrieved from <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/meaning-normativity/">https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/meaning-normativity/</ext-link>.
Hoffmann, T., & Trousdale, G. (Eds.). (2013). <em>The Oxford handbook of construction grammar</em>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396683.001.0001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396683.001.0001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Huddleston, R., & Pullum, G. K. (2002). <em>The Cambridge grammar of the English language</em>. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/3738393" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.2307/3738393</a>">https://doi.org/10.2307/3738393</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.2307/3738393" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.2307/3738393</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Itkonen, E. (2008). The central role of normativity in language and linguistics. In J. Zlatev, T.P. Racine, C. Sinha & E. Itkonen (Eds.), <em>The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity</em> (pp. 279–305). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/celcr.12.16itk" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1075/celcr.12.16itk</a>">https://doi.org/10.1075/celcr.12.16itk</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/celcr.12.16itk" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/celcr.12.16itk</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Krifka, M., Pelletier, F. J., Carlson, G. N., terMeulen, A., Link, G., & Chierchia, G. (1995). Genericity: An introduction. In G. N. Carlson & F. J. Pelletier (Eds.), <em>The generic book</em> (pp. 1–124). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Langacker, R. W. (1997). Generics and habituals. In A. Athanasiadou & R. Dirven (Eds.), <em>On conditionals again</em> (pp. 191–222). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.143.11lan" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/cilt.143.11lan</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Leslie, S.-J. (2015). “Hillary Clinton is the only man in the Obama administration”: Dual character concepts, generics, and gender. <em>Analytic Philosophy</em>, <em>56</em>(2), 111–141. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/phib.12063" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1111/phib.12063</a>">https://doi.org/10.1111/phib.12063</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1111/phib.12063" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1111/phib.12063</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Mäkilähde, A., Leppänen, V., & Itkonen, E. (Eds.). (2019). <em>Norms and normativity in language and linguistics</em>. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.209.01mak" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.209.01mak</a>">https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.209.01mak</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.209.01mak" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/slcs.209.01mak</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Nickel, B. (2016). <em>Between logic and the world: An integrated theory of generics</em>. Oxford: Oxford University Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199640003.001.0001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199640003.001.0001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Nuyts, J., Byloo, P., & Diepeveen, J. (2010). On deontic modality, directivity, and mood: The case of Dutch <em>mogen</em> and <em>moeten</em>. <em>Journal of Pragmatics</em>, <em>42</em>(1), 16–34. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2009.05.012" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2009.05.012</a>">https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2009.05.012</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2009.05.012" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.pragma.2009.05.012</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Paulus, M., & Schmidt, M. F. H. (2018). The early development of the normative mind. <em>Journal of Experimental Child Psychology</em>, <em>165</em>, 1–6. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.011" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.011</a>">https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.011</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.011" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.011</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Pelletier, F. J., & Asher, N. (1997). Generics and defaults. In J. van Benthem & A. ter Meulen (Eds.), <em>Handbook of logic and language</em> (pp. 1125–1179). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181822ebd" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181822ebd</a>">https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181822ebd</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181822ebd" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181822ebd</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Radden, G. (2009). Generic reference in English. A metonymic and conceptual blending theory. In K.-U. Panther, L. T. Thornburg & A. Barcelona (Eds.), <em>Metonymy and metaphor in grammar</em> (pp. 199–228). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1075/hcp.25.13rad" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1075/hcp.25.13rad</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schmid, H.-J., & Günther, F. (2016). Toward a unified socio-cognitive framework for salience in language. <em>Frontiers in Psychology</em>, <em>7</em>, 1110.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01110" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01110</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Schmidt, M. F. H., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Developing an understanding of normativity. In A. Newen, L. De Bruin & S. Gallagher (Eds.), <em>The Oxford handbook of cognition: Embodied, embedded, enactive and extended</em> (pp. 685–706). <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.36" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.36</a>">https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.36</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.36" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.36</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Shackelford, T. K., & Weekes-Shackelford, V. A. (Eds.). (2012). <em>The Oxford handbook of evolutionary perspectives on violence, homicide, and war</em>. New York: Oxford University Press. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738403.001.0001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738403.001.0001</a>">https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738403.001.0001</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738403.001.0001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738403.001.0001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Turiel, E. (2008). Thought about actions in social domains: Morality, social conventions, and social interactions. <em>Cognitive Development</em>, <em>23</em>(1), 136–154. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.04.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.04.001</a>">https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.04.001</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.04.001" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.04.001</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Wedgwood, R. (2018). The unity of normativity. In D. Star (Ed.), <em>The Oxford handbook of reasons and normativity</em> (pp. 23–45). Oxford: Oxford University Press. <ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" xlink:href="<a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199657889.013.2" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199657889.013.2</a>">https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199657889.013.2</ext-link><dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199657889.013.2" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199657889.013.2</a></dgdoi:pub-id>
Young, P. (2003). The power of norms. In P. Hammerstein (Ed.), <em>Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation</em> (pp. 389–399). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<dgdoi:pub-id xmlns:dgdoi="http://degruyter.com/resources/doi-from-crossref" pub-id-type="doi"><a href="https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/3232.003.0022" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer" class="text-signal-blue hover:underline">10.7551/mitpress/3232.003.0022</a></dgdoi:pub-id>